DAUER BEHAVIOR
Click image for closeup view Click pictures for new window with figure and legend, click again for high resolution image

1 General Description

Dauer larvae display characteristic behaviors that maximize their survival and dispersal to new, more hospitable, environments. Dauer-characteristic behaviors have been reported for sensory behaviors, locomotion, feeding and nictation behavior.



2 Sensory Behaviors

2.1 Chemosensory Responses

Chemosensory attraction is a major determinant for host tracking by parasitic nematodes in the infective and dispersal stages, which are analogous to the dauer stage in C. elegans (Ashton et al., 1999; O’Halloran and Brunell, 2003; Zhao et al., 2007). Nematodes can respond to both dissolved chemicals and volatile gasses. C. elegans dauers respond normally to many of the chemical attractants detected during the non-dauer stages (Ward, 1973; Cassada and Russell, 1975). However, Albert and Riddle (1983) reported that dauers failed to respond to sodium ions (Na+), which is a potent attractant for nondauers. Initiation of dauer recovery, by exposure to food, restored Na+ attraction, suggesting that chemotactic sensitivity to this attractant is impeded by the dauer’s cuticular features, such as the the buccal plug.

Carbon dioxide (CO2) is a volatile odorant that is widely used for parasitic host tracking. In C. elegans, bimodal responses to CO2 are determined by the dauer arrest decision. Dauer larvae display attraction to CO2, while CO2 acts as a repellent to directly-developing larvae and adults (Bretscher et al., 2008; Hallem and Sternberg, 2008; Hallem et al., 2011). Attraction to CO2 is also conserved among infective larval forms of two insect parasitic nematode species, Heterorhabditis bacteriophora and Steinernema carpocapsae (Hallem et al., 2011). In all 3 nematode species, CO2 attraction is mediated, at least in part, by the BAG neurons of the nose (Hallem et al., 2011). Genetic analysis revealed that CO2 avoidance by C. elegans nondauers requires ets-5 which encodes an ETS-transcription factor acting in the BAG neurons (Guillermin et al., 2011).


2.2 Thermosensory Responses

Dauers may have increased thermal sensitivity compared to nondauers.  Warm temperature (27oC) leads to higher rates of dauer formation in wildtype animals (Ailion and Thomas, 2000).  In addition, many genetic mutations with dauer-formation (Daf) phenotypes are temperature-sensitive, causing dauer arrest at 25oC but allowing normal development at lower temperatures (Gems et al., 1998).  However, dauers have been shown to have dramatically reduced responses to noxious thermal signals (Wittenburg and Baumeister, 1999).  Dauer animals fail to respond to noxious heat, whereas when dauers later recover to the L4 or adult stages, their response to noxious heat reverts to normal.  These noxious signals are sensed by the highly branched nociceptor neurons in the cuticle, PVD and FLP (Chatzigeorgiou et al., 2010; Hall and Treinin, 2011).

Thermosensitivity is an important aspect of the life cycle for Strongyloides stercoralis parasitic nematodes.  The infective juvenile, a 3rd larval stage analogous to the C. elegans dauer, migrates towards warm temperatures, which may signify the presence of a susceptible host animal.  Neural ablation experiments show that thermosensitivity in infective juveniles requires a pair of amphid head neurons (Lopez et al., 2000).  These neurons may be analogous to the AFD finger neurons in C. elegans. In dauers, the AFD neurons may have more elaborated finger-like endings than nondauers (Albert and Riddle, 1983), although this feature has not been consistently observed (Procko et al., 2011). 



3 Locomotion

Dauer larvae have a tendency to remain motionless (Cassada and Russell, 1975; Gaglia and Kenyon, 2009) in either a rod-like or curved position (Gems et al., 1998, Lee et al., 2011). For a comparison of locomotion in dauers and L2/L3 larvae, see DBehaviorVID 1 and DBehaviorVID 2, respectively. Pharynx pumping also ceases in dauers. In these ways, dauer behavior is reminiscent of larval behaviors during the intermolt lethargus periods (Cassada and Russell, 1975). Mechanical stimuli, such as jarring the medium or touching the animal directly, induce an immediate and fast-paced locomotory response as the dauer flees the area (Cassada and Russell, 1975). This rapid locomotory response shows that the dauer’s body muscles are fully functional and that the tendency of dauers to lie motionless may reflect a shift in neuromuscular signaling, rather than a decline in muscle functional capacity. Curiously, conditions that alter the level of dopamine signaling (either up or down) can stimulate movement in dauer larvae, without altering baseline locomotion in adult animals (Gaglia and Kenyon, 2009). This evidence points to dopaminergic signaling as potential factor regulating locomotory behavior in the dauer.

DBehaviorVID 1: Dauer larvae locomotory behaviors. Dauer larvae on an agar surface lie motionless for much of the time. One larva moving through the field demonstrates the characteristic exaggerated sinusoidal behavior. (Video Source: Mark Alkema, University of Massachusetts Medical School.) To open high resolution video in a separate page click here. (Depending on connection speed, video may take several minutes to load.)
DBehaviorVID 2: Non-dauer larvae locomotory behaviors. In contrast to the motionless tendency of dauer larvae, non-dauer larvae are observed to move nearly constantly in the presence of food. Feeding larvae may or may not cover large distances when locomotory. Source: Mark Alkema, University of Massachusetts Medical School. To open high resolution video in a separate page, click here. (Depending on connection speed, video may take several minutes to load.)

4 Nictation

Dauers are sometimes observed performing a unique behavior called nictation. Nictating dauers have an erect posture, balancing on the end of their tails with their heads swaying in the air (Cassada and Russell, 1975) (DBehaviorFIG 1, DBehaviorVID 3, VID 4 and VID 5). Nictation behavior is specific to dauers and is not performed by directly-developing larvae or adults. On laboratory growth medium, C. elegans dauers are frequently found nictating upon filamentous structures, such as fungal hyphae, which commonly contaminate the agar growth medium. Dauers may also nictate as a mass, forming a structure resembling a waving stalk upon the substrate (DBehaviorVID 4) (Félix and Duveau, 2012). Controlled conditions for dauer nictation utilize artificial nictation substrates, such as medical cotton gauze and polydimethylsiloxane (PDMS)-based microfluidic structures (Lee et al., 2011).

DBehaviorFIG 1: Larval nictation and flipping behaviors for dispersal
DBehaviorFIG 1: Dauers performing nictation behavior. C. elegans dauers performing nictation behavior in the wild from the tips of pointed moss leaf tips. In the lower-left of the field, a single dauer is nictating alone. A large mass of dauers nictating together is shown in the center and right regions of the field. (Image source: Marie-Anne Félix, Institute of Biology of École Normale Supérieure.)
DBehaviorVID 3: Nictating C. elegans dauer larvae. Nictating dauers can be seen as objects waving upon filamentous nictation substrates, which may be debris or fungi accumulated on the agar surface. At the bottom-center of the viewer field, a group of dauers has become entwined together to form a larger nictating object. In addition, single dauers are nictating in other regions of the viewer field, including the left side. (Video source: Nathan Schroeder, Rutgers University.) Click "play" arrow to start video. To open high resolution video in a separate page click here. (Depending on connection speed, video may take several minutes to load.)
DBehaviorVID 4: A mass of thousands of dauers exhibiting nictation behavior. Dauers can be seen waving individually or in groups at the surface of any pointed structure. In this movie, thousands of dauers are observed oscillating en masse as found in the wild on a rotting apple (Félix and Duveau, 2012). This nictation behavior is thought to help in finding an invertebrate host for migration. (Video source: Marie-Anne Félix,Institute of Biology of École Normale Supérieure.) Click "play" arrow to start video. To open high resolution video in a separate page click here. (Depending on connection speed, video may take several minutes to load.)
DBehaviorVID 5: Dauers exhibiting nictation behavior along the top of an agar surface. In this movie, individual dauers are observed slowly waving back and forth. They appear to be standing on their tails and are waving their whole bodies in the air. The motion of the dauers is much slower compared to what is seen when they swimming in liquid and is different in waveform than when worms are crawling on a plate or swimming in liquid. Movie is in real time. (Video source: Mark Alkema, Universityof Massachusettes Medical School.) To open high resolution video in a separate page, click on video picture above or click here. (Depending on connection speed, video may take several minutes to load.)

Nictation requires cholinergic signaling within the ciliated IL2 neurons (Lee et al., 2011). In C. elegans dauers, the ciliated endings of the IL2 neurons are covered by a thick body wall cuticle over the animal’s nose (see DCutFIG 5), and the distal channel to the exterior is filled or narrowed with excess dense material (Albert and Riddle, 1981; DHH, unpublished observations), possibly interfering with transmission of chemical cues that might be related to nictation. Even in wild type adults, the IL2 cilia are somewhat difficult to dye-fill (Tong and Burglin, 2010), suggesting that the IL2 cilium is less exposed to the exterior than other chemosensors. Rather, IL2 neurons may initiate nictation in response to mechanosensory stimuli indicating appropriate environmental substrates for nictation (Lee et al., 2011).  Nictation initiation is a dauer-specific function of IL2s, since activation of IL2s was not able to trigger nictation in non-dauers. In addition to mechanical stimulation of nictation, photostimulation with electric lights can trigger nictation behavior in starved populations of C. elegans (Staniland, 1957). 

Nematodes demonstrate two strategies for foraging and dispersal. Nictation behavior exemplifies the ambushing, or “sit-and-wait”, strategy of host tracking and dispersal. The alternate dispersal mechanism utilizes cruising, or “widely-foraging”, in which nematodes actively explore their environment for a suitable food or hosts. While ambushing allows for targeting of quickly moving prey, (Huey and Pianka, 1981; Campbell and Gaugler, 1993), cruising is optimized for searching out sedentary, non-motile prey, such as rotting fruits.

Nematodes in the Steinernema genus demonstrate an unusual variant of ambushing in which nictation is the first step of a more complex dispersal behavior. From a nictation position, an infective juvenile can bend backwards, progressively looping its body until the tail is flung away from the substrate, propelling the entire body towards the oncoming host (Campbell and Kaya, 1999; Hallem et al., 2011) (DBehaviorFIG 2). Jumping behavior in S. carpocapsae infective juveniles is stimulated by the presence of the nematode’s host, honeycomb moth larvae, but not by a non-specific cue, such as an air puff (Campbell and Kaya, 1999). Carbon dioxide gas can also stimulate S. carpocapsae jumping behavior in a BAG-dependent manner (Hallem et al., 2011).

DBehaviorFIG 2: Larval nictation and flipping behaviors for dispersal. A. Nematodes in the dauer or infective stages are adapted for dispersal to new environments in search of fresh food sources or for attaching to a new host, respectively. Both needs require mechanisms for attaching to a vector or host animal, such as a bird or insect. B-D. Larvae perform nictation behavior to increase their height above the surface, increasing the chances of being picked up by a passing object, such as the birds claw or an insects body. E. S. carpocapsae infective juveniles also perform jumping behavior to become airborne, thereby acquiring a more secure attachment to the passing animal.

5 Pharyngeal Pumping

The dauer animal has modified body structures to prevent normal feeding behavior, including a closed mouth and a somewhat withered pharynx. The pharyngeal muscles virtually cease pumping activity during dauer, although those muscles retain contractility. The pharyngeal muscles show altered responses to sensory cues, compared to other stages (Keane and Avery, 2003). In normal animals at any other larval stage or adulthood, light tail tap causes a brief halt (for several seconds) to the normal steady pumping of the pharynx when the animal is on food (Chalfie et al., 1985). A harsh tail tap in adults leads to a much longer period of inhibition, from which the animal only recovers slowly (Keane and Avery, 2003). Curiously, a harsh tail tap in dauers causes a brief stimulation of pharyngeal pumping (Keane and Avery, 2003). Both inhibitory and stimulatory responses are sensitive to mutations in unc-7, suggesting that gap junctions lie somewhere in the ill-defined behavioral pathway between the tail and the pharynx.



6 References

Ailion, M. and Thomas, J.H. 2000. Dauer formation induced by high temperatures in Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics.156: 1047-67. Article

Albert, P.S. and Riddle, D.L. 1983. Developmental alterations in sensory neuroanatomy of the Caenorhabditis elegans dauer larva. J. Comp. Neurol. 219: 461-481. Abstract

Ashton, F.T., Li, J., and Schad, G.A. 1999. Chemo- and thermosensory neurons: structure and function in animal parasitic nematodes. Vet. Parasitol. 84: 297-316. Abstract

Bretscher, A.J., Busch, K.E., and de Bono, M. 2008. A carbon dioxide avoidance behavior is integrated with responses to ambient oxygen and food in Caenorhabditis elegans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 105: 8044-8049. Article

Campbell, J.F. and Gaugler, R. 1993. Nictation behaviour and its ecological implications in the host search strategies of entomopathogenic nematodes (Heterorhabditidae and Steinernematidae). Behaviour 126:155-169. Abstract

Campbell, J.F. and Kaya, H.K. 1999. How and why a parasitic nematode jumps. Nature 397: 485-486. Abstract

Cassada, R.C. and Russell, R.L. 1975. The dauer larva, a post-embryonic developmental variant of the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. Dev. Biol. 46: 326-342. Abstract

Chalfie, M., Sulston, J.E., White, J.G., Southgate, E., Thomson, J.N. and Brenner, S. 1985. The neural circuit for touch sensitivity in Caenorhabditis elegans. J. Neurosci. 5: 956-964. Article

Chatzigeorgiou, M., Yoo, S., Watson, J.D., Lee, W.H., Spencer, W.C., Kindt, K.S., Hwang, S.W., Miller, D.M. 3rd, Treinin, M., Driscoll, M. and Schafer, W.R. 2010. Specific roles for DEG/ENaC and TRP channels in touch and thermosensation in C. elegans nociceptors. Nat. Neurosci. 13: 861-8. Article

Félix, M.-A. and Duveau, F. 2012. Population dynamics and habitat sharing of natural populations of Caenorhabditis elegans and C. briggsae. BMC Biology 10: 59. Article

Gaglia, M.M. and Kenyon, C. 2009. Stimulation of movement in a quiescent, hibernation form of C. elegans by dopamine signaling. J. Neurosci. 29: 7302-7314. Article

Gems, D., Sutton, A.J., Sundermeyer, M.L., Albert, P.S., King, K.V., Edgley, M.L, Larsen, P.L., and Riddle, D.L. 1998. Two pleiotropic classes of daf-2 mutation affect larval arrest, adult behavior, reproduction and longevity in Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics 150: 129-155. Article

Guillermin, M.L., Castelletto, M.L., Hallem, E.A. 2011. Differentiation of carbon dioxide-sensing neurons in Caenorhabditis elegans requires the ETS-5 transcription factor. Genetics 189: 1327-1339. Abstract

Hall, D.H. and Treinin, M. 2011. How does morphology relate to function in sensory arbors? Trends Neurosci. 34: 443-51. Abstract

Hallem, E.A. and Sternberg, P.W. 2008. Acute carbon dioxide avoidance in Caenorhabditis elegans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 105: 8038-8043. Article

Hallem, E.A., Dillman, A.R., Hong, A.V., Zhang, Y., Yano, J.M., DeMarco, S.F., and Sternberg, P.W. 2011. A sensory code for host seeking in parasitic nematodes. Curr. Biol. 21: 377-383. Article

Huey, R.B. and Pianka, E.R. 1981. Ecological consequences of foraging mode. Ecology 62: 991-999. Article

Keane, J. and Avery, L. 2003. Mechanosensory inputs influenceCaenorhabditis elegans pharyngeal activity via ivermectin sensitivity genes. Genetics 164: 153-162. Article

Lee, H., Choi, M., Lee, D., Kim, H., Hwang, H., Kim, H., Park, S., Paik, Y., and Lee, J. 2011. Nictation, a dispersal behavior of the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans, is regulated by IL2 neurons. Nature Neurosci. 15: 107-112. Abstract

Lopez, P.M., Boston, R., Ashton, F.T., and Schad, G.A. 2000. The neurons of class ALD mediate thermotaxis in the parasitic nematode, Strongyloides stercoralis. Int. J. Parasitol. 30: 1115-1121. Abstract

O’Halloran, D.M. and Burnell, A.M. 2003. An investigation of chemotaxis in the insect parasitic nematode Heterorhabditis bacteriophora. Parasitology 127: 375-385. Abstract

Procko, C., Lu, Y. and Shaham, S. 2011. Glia delimit shape changes of sensory neuron receptive endings in C. elegans. Development. 138:1371-1381. Article

Staniland, L.N. 1957. The swarming of Rhabditid eelworms in mushroom houses. Plant Pathol. 6: 61-62. Abstract

Tong, Y-G. and Burglin, T.R. 2010. Conditions for dye-filling of sensory neurons in Caenorhabditis elegans. J. Neurosci. Methods 188: 58-61. Abstract

Ward, S. 1973. Chemotaxis by the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans: Identification of attractants and analysis of the response by use of mutants. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. 70: 817-821. Article

Wittenburg, W. and R. Baumeister. 1999. Thermal avoidance in Caenorhabditis elegans: An approach to the study of nociception. PNAS 96: 10477-82. Article

Zhao, L.L., Wei, W., Kang, L., and Sun, J.H. 2007. Chemotaxis of the pinewood nematode, Bursaphelenchus xylophilus, to volatiles associated with host pine, Pinus massoniana, and its vector Monochamus alternatus. J. Chem. Ecol. 33: 1207-1216. Abstract



This chapter should be cited as: Wolkow, C.A. and Hall, D.H. 2011. Dauer Behavior. In WormAtlas. doi:10.3908/wormatlas.3.3
Edited for the web by Laura A. Herndon. Last revision: February 7, 2014.

We would like to acknowledge and thank Nathan Schroeder for his contributions and suggestions for this section.

image
 
image